Evolution
of Religions
In
The Name of The Evolver
Mankind's
nearest living relatives are normal chimpanzees and bonobos. These
primates share a typical precursor with people who lived in the
vicinity of six and eight million years prior. It is thus that
chimpanzees and bonobos are seen as the best accessible surrogate for
this normal precursor. Barbara Lord contends that while non-human
primates are not religious, they do show a few characteristics that
would have been important for the development of religion. These
attributes incorporate high insight, a limit with respect to
representative correspondence, a feeling of social standards,
acknowledgment of "self" and an idea of continuity. There
is uncertain confirmation that Homo neanderthalensis may have covered
their dead which is proof of the utilization of custom. The
utilization of entombment customs is believed to be proof of
religious action, and there is no other confirmation that religion
existed in human culture before people achieved behavioral modernity.
There
is almost certainly that numerous creatures encounter rich and
profound feelings. It's not a matter of if feelings have advanced in
creatures yet why they have developed as they have. We should always
remember that our feelings are the endowments of our predecessors,
our creature family. We have sentiments thus do different creatures.
Among
the diverse feelings that creatures show unmistakably and
unambiguously is distress. Numerous creatures show significant sorrow
at the misfortune or nonattendance of a dear companion or cherished
one. Nobel laureate ethologist Konrad Lorenz states: "A greylag
goose that has lost its accomplice demonstrates every one of the
manifestations that [developmental psychologist] John Bowlby has
portrayed in youthful human youngsters in his popular book Newborn
child Sadness . . . the eyes sink profound into their attachments,
and the individual has a general hanging background, actually giving
the head a chance to hang . . ." Ocean lion moms, watching their
children being eaten by executioner whales, howl pathetically,
anguishing their misfortune. Dolphins have been seen attempting to
spare a dead newborn child and grieve a short time later. Stories
about sorrow stricken friend creatures flourish; see moreover).
Wild
creatures likewise lament. Among the best illustrations are lamenting
customs of elephants in the wild saw by such famous analysts as Iain
Douglas-Hamilton, Cynthia Greenery and Joyce Poole. Hostage elephants
additionally lament; see too. To cite Joyce Poole: "As I viewed
Tonie's vigil over her dead infant, I got my first extremely solid
inclination that elephants lament. I will always remember the
demeanor all over, her eyes, her mouth, the way she conveyed her
ears, her head, and her body. All aspects of her spelled sadness".
Youthful elephants who saw their moms being murdered frequently wake
up shouting.
Cynthia
Greenery portrays the activities of the individuals from an elephant
family above after a gathering part had been shot: "Teresia and
Trista ended up plainly frenzied and stooped down and attempted to
lift her up. They worked their tusks under her back and under her
head. At a certain point they prevailing with regards to lifting her
into a sitting position however her body slumped down. Her family had
a go at everything to energize her, kicking and tusking her, and
Tullulah even went off and gathered a trunkful of grass and attempted
to stuff it in her mouth."
Iain
Douglas-Hamilton and his associates have demonstrated that elephants
stretch out this empathy to nonrelatives, to the individuals who
aren't hereditarily related, and no less than one tale indicates them
extending it to people. A news report recounted an elephant in
northern Kenya that stomped a human mother and her youngster and
afterward halted to cover them before vanishing in the shrubbery.
Elephants don't indicate concern only for their own kinfolk, or their
own particular kind, yet rather elephants demonstrate a general worry
for the situation of others.
Nonhuman
primates additionally lament the loss of others. Gana, a hostage
gorilla, unmistakably lamented the loss of her newborn child and the
picture of her conveying her dead infant was appeared far and wide.
Jane Goodall watched Rock, a youthful chimpanzee, pull back from his
gathering, quit eating, and pass on of a broken heart after the
demise of his mom, Flo. Here is Goodall's portrayal from her book
Through a Window:
"Never
should I overlook looking as, three days after Flo's demise, Stone
climbed gradually into a tall tree close to the stream. He strolled
along one of the branches, at that point ceased and stood unmoving,
gazing down at a void home. After around two minutes he dismissed
and, with the developments of an old man, descended, strolled a
couple of steps, at that point lay, wide eyes looking forward. The
home was one which he and Flo had shared a brief time before Flo
passed on. . . . within the sight of his enormous sibling [Figan],
[Flint] had appeared to shake off a tad bit of his melancholy. Be
that as it may, at that point he all of a sudden left the gathering
and hustled back to where Flo had passed on and there sank into ever
more profound wretchedness. . . . Rock turned out to be progressively
torpid, declined nourishment and, with his safe framework
consequently debilitated, fell wiped out. The last time I saw him
alive, he was empty looked at, emaciated and totally discouraged,
clustered in the vegetation near where Flo had kicked the bucket. . .
. the last short adventure he made, delaying to rest each couple of
feet, was to the very place where Flo's body had lain. There he
remained for a few hours, in some cases gazing and gazing into the
water. He battled on somewhat further, at that point nestled
into—never moved again."
Another
account of lamenting chimpanzees as of late was accounted for in the
Day by day Mail.
Gorillas
are known to hold wakes for dead companions, something that a few
zoos have formalized in a service when one of their gorillas passes
away. Donna Fernandes, now leader of the Wild ox Zoo, recounts the
narrative of being at Boston's Franklin Stop Zoo ten years back amid
the wake for a female gorilla, Babs, who had kicked the bucket of
disease. She depicts seeing the gorilla's long-term mate say
farewell: "He was wailing and slamming his chest,...
furthermore, he got a bit of her most loved nourishment — celery —
and place it in her grasp and attempted to inspire her to wake up. I
was sobbing, it was so passionate." Later, the scene at Babs'
December burial service was likewise moving. As revealed by
neighborhood news, gorilla relatives "one by one ... recorded
into" the room where "Babs' body lay," moving toward
their "dearest pioneer" and "delicately sniffing the
body."
Whenever
Sylvia, a primate, lost Sierra, her nearest prepping accomplice and
little girl to a lion, she reacted in a way that would be viewed as
exceptionally human-like: she sought companions for help. Said Anne
Engh, a specialist in he College of Pennsylvania's Division of
Science. "With Sierra out of the picture, Sylvia experienced
what could just truly be depicted as misery, comparing with an
expansion in her glucocorticoid levels."
Jim
and Jamie Dutcher depict the melancholy and grieving in a wolf pack
after the loss of the low-positioning omega female wolf, Motaki, to a
mountain lion. The pack lost their soul and their perkiness. They
never again cried as a gathering, yet rather they "sang alone in
a moderate forlorn cry." They were discouraged — tails and
heads held low and strolling delicately and gradually — when they
happened upon where Motaki was murdered. They reviewed the zone and
stuck their ears back and dropped their tails, a motion that
ordinarily implies accommodation. It took around a month and a half
for the pack to come back to ordinary. The Dutchers likewise recount
a wolf pack in Canada in which one pack part passed on and the others
meandered about in a figure eight as though looking for her. They
additionally cried long and forlornly. Foxes additionally have been
watched performing memorial service ceremonies.
"Llamas
are gregarious by nature, to a great degree discerning, and produce
profound bonds with each other. In the field, our llamas frequently
encourage in a similar range, rest by each other, and remain nearby
together when they go head to head a new creature or predator. On the
trail, they turn out to be to a great degree unsettled on the off
chance that they dismiss each other when one stops to rest and falls
behind. They vocalize a considerable amount. My most loved is their
fragile welcome call, which sounds like a little bagpipe breathing
out. At the point when my family moved from Colorado to The Frozen
North, we carried our two Colorado llamas with us. As destiny would
have it, we acquired two The Frozen North llamas with our new house
and grounds. Every twosome had spent their lives together. At in the
first place, the twosomes were somewhat standoffish, however in time,
they turned out to be quick companions and a foursome. Quite a while
later, the most seasoned llama, Boone, passed on abruptly at
twenty-seven years of age. One day, he set down on his side,
excessively feeble, making it impossible to get up. The following
day, his life accomplice, Bridger, kicked the bucket in a similar
manner, beside him. It was late-winter and the ground was as yet
solidified, so we procured a companion with an escavator to set up
their grave directly over the fence. We painstakingly raised Boone
and Bridger over the fence and into the ground, at that point secured
them. The other match, Taffy and Pumpernickel, remained by and viewed
the whole procedure unobtrusively. For the following two days, stoic
Taffy remained over the fence from the grave and gazed at the opening
in the ground. She scarcely moved from the spot. Volatile
Pumpernickel remained in his little stable and cried for two days. On
the third day, they rose up out of their lamenting and continued
their typical exercises. Did Bridger surrender himself to death
following the loss of his long lasting mate Boone? Furthermore, Taffy
and Pumpernickel, both exceptionally unmistakable identities,
lamented in their very own ways. For me, the most moving memory of
losing two llamas so near one another was encountering the minding
and agreeable llama passing and lamenting procedure."
Jaybirds
likewise lament the loss of different jaybirds; see too. I as of late
got this story by means of email in light of the papers about my
perceptions of jaybird sorrow. "I have a homestead in Bolton, UK
and we were invade with Jaybirds. The response from the jaybirds [to
the body of another magpie] in the region was likened to a scene from
the film 'The Winged creatures', as they encompassed the dead fowl
and attempted to stir it with their bills. When they achieved the
conclusion that it was without a doubt dead, there was an overflowing
of noisy snickering clamors which came to a significant crescendo
(there were around 20 of them); this was resounded by a comparable
thoughtful melody from an adjacent wood and inside a moment, from all
encompassing zones giving the feeling that several jaybirds were
being recounted the demise and all the while communicating their
despondency. It was very frightening and I stayed inside the
protected bounds of an outbuilding until the point when all was
finished."
Why
do creatures lament and why do we see misery in various types of
creatures? It's been recommended that sorrow responses may take into
consideration the reshuffling of status connections or the filling
the conceptive opportunity left by the expired, or for encouraging
congruity of the gathering. Some conjecture that maybe grieving
fortifies social bonds among the survivors who gather as one to pay
their last regards. This may upgrade aggregate union when it's
probably going to be debilitated.
Sadness
in creatures: It's presumptuous to think that we are the only ones
who worship.
Elephants
really exhibit ceremonies around their expired, which incorporates
long stretches of quiet and grieving at the purpose of death and a
procedure of coming back to grave destinations and stroking the
remains. An imperative region of science includes researching the
beginnings in creatures of characteristics that are thought of as
exceptionally human. One way that people seem one of a kind is in the
significance they join to the dead groups of different people,
especially those of their nearby family, and the customs that they
have produced for covering them. Conversely, most creatures seem to
indicate just restricted enthusiasm for the cadavers or related stays
of dead people of their own species. African elephants (Loxodonta
africana) are strange in that they not just give sensational
responses to the dead groups of different elephants, but at the same
time are accounted for to deliberately explore elephant bones and
tusks that they experience, and it has infrequently been proposed
that they visit the bones of relatives.
(Image:
Royal Society/Karen McComb)
Elephants
therefore pay reverence to the bones of dead relatives in their home
ranges, an investigation of the animals' reactions to skulls and
ivory recommends.
People
separated, just a couple of creatures demonstrate any enthusiasm for
their own particular dead. Chimpanzees indicate drawn out and complex
practices towards a dead social accomplice – however relinquish
them once the cadaver begins decaying. However, lions, for instance,
may sniff or lick a dead individual from its own particular species
previously continuing to eat up the body.
African
elephants have been seen to end up plainly profoundly unsettled when
they run over the assemblages of their own, and they have been
believed to give careful consideration to the skull and ivory of
long-dead elephants. Notwithstanding, this intrigue had not been
tried tentatively.
Presently
inquire about from a group in the UK and Kenya has exhibited that
African elephants pay a more elevated amount important to elephant
skulls contrasted and those of different creatures and ivory
contrasted with wood.
Nonetheless,
the group couldn't prove stories that elephants particularly visit
the bones of dead relatives. The elephant families in their
investigation were not able choose the skull of their dead female
authority from other families' dead matrons.
"In
any case, their enthusiasm for the ivory and skulls of their own
species implies that they would probably visit the bones of relatives
who kick the bucket inside their home range," composes the
group, lead by Karen McComb at the College of Sussex, UK.
Extensive
brains, seemingly perpetual
"It
makes one wonder why do they do this? This enthusiasm for stays of
creatures, long-dead, hasn't generally been seen in some other
species separated from people," McComb disclosed to New
Researcher.
She
likewise noticed that recorded enthusiasm for the dead has been found
in elephants and chimps – "two extremely social species, with
very mind boggling structures, substantial brains and who are
seemingly perpetual". She theorizes: "It might be
associated with specific subjective capacities or parts of social
conduct."
"Elephants
are exceptionally savvy and profoundly material creatures," says
David Field, head of creature watch over London and Whipsnade Zoos in
the UK. "The reality they can recognize their own skulls and
those of different species is not shocking."
"Elephants
themselves are a matriarchal society loaded with aunts and relatives
who include close bonds inside a gathering," he includes. A
demise in the family may be a critical get-together. "It could
affect social holding and structure inside the gathering," he
disclosed to New Researcher.
Noticing
and touching
McComb
and associates examined African elephants (Loxodonta africana) living
in Amboseli National Stop, Kenya. Groups of elephants were given
protests by putting them around 25 meters far from the closest
elephant and after that heading out and watching the response of
creatures.
In
one examination, 17 families were given skulls from an elephant, a
wild ox and a rhinoceros. The elephants demonstrated impressive
enthusiasm for the skull of their own species. They did this by
noticing and touching individual items with their trunks and once in
a while touching them gently with their feet.
In
another examination, 19 families were given an elephant skull, a bit
of ivory and a bit of wood. The animals demonstrated a solid
inclination for the skull over the other two items, and for the ivory
over the wood.
The
third examination tried three elephant families who had as of late
lost the leader of their family. Every wa gave three skulls of female
authorities including their own – however they didn't demonstrate
an inclination for their relative's skull.
The
thought of elephant burial grounds – where old elephants stray to
bite the dust – has been uncovered as myth by past examinations,
the scientists note. Regardless, they trust their examinations "cast
light" on why elephants are regularly observed interfacing with
the skulls and ivory of dead associates.
However,
there is no real way to tell whether the elephants are grieving their
dead – despite the fact that they get extremely energized when
moving toward bodies, with emissions spilling from their sanctuaries.
Diary
reference: Science Letters (DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0400)
Here,
we utilize precise introductions of protest exhibits to show that
African elephants demonstrate more elevated amounts of enthusiasm for
elephant skulls and ivory than in normal items or the skulls of other
expansive earthly well evolved creatures. In any case, they don't
appear to explicitly choose the skulls of their own relatives for
examination with the goal that visits to dead relatives most likely
outcome from a more broad fascination in elephant remains.
During
human evolution, the hominid brain tripled in size, peaking 500,000
years ago. Much of the brain's expansion took place in the neocortex.
This part of the brain is involved in processing higher order
cognitive functions that are connected with human religiosity. Dr
Narinda proposes that numerous species lament passing and loss.
The
two main schools of thought of Dr Narinda hold that either religion
evolved due to natural selection and has selective advantage, or that
religion is an evolutionary byproduct of other mental adaptations, Dr
Narinda says. Social living can take one of two forms: animals can
form loose aggregations (exemplified by insect swarms and antelope
herds, which are typically based on short-term advantages and whose
persistence depends on immediate costs and benefits) or they can form
congregations (exemplified by the bonded social groups of primates
and some other mammals, including cetaceans, elephants and equids
among others) whose advantages derive from long-term association, and
whose persistence mainly depends on the trade-off between short- and
long-term benefits and costs . It has been suggested that this second
kind of sociality is a kind of large-scale coordination problem that
depends on bonding processes underpinned by more sophisticated
cognitive mechanisms (associated with larger brains ). Indeed, the
social brain hypothesis implies that the size of group that can
coordinate its behaviour is limited by the cognitive capacity
(essentially, the neural processing capacity) that organisms can
bring to bear on the problem.Individual religious belief utilizes
reason based in the neocortex and often varies from collective
religion.
Religious
ideas can be traced to the evolution of brains large enough to make
possible the kind of abstract thought necessary to formulate
religious and philosophical ideas. Not only do our brains permit us
to outwit predators and colleagues and solve extraordinarily
difficult problems; they also create prob-lems by endowing us with
intense consciousness and knowledge of our own mortality. This
produces a load of anxiety that some feel is unbearable and that all
of us attempt to find some way to reconcile. As the saying goes, life
is tough, and then you die. In the face of life's tribulations,
people seek a belief system that appears to justify those
tribulations. Those who find such a system of orientation, it seems,
tend to enjoy not only better mental health but also better physical
health. Beliefs in the supernatural clearly have had and
continue to have enormous influence on human behavior and the
evolution of human societies. An examination of why religions
evolved, the roles of religion, and the evolution of those roles is
critical to understanding human natures and can be carried out
without reference to the "truth" or "falsity" of
the claims of any religion. Sociality is primarily a coordination
problem. However, the social (or communication) complexity hypothesis
suggests that the kinds of information that can be acquired and
processed may limit the size and/or complexity of social groups that
a species can maintain, i.e. successive increases in the kinds of
information processed allow organisms to break through the glass
ceilings that otherwise limit the size of social groups: larger
groups can only be achieved at the cost of more sophisticated kinds
of information processing that are disadvantageous when optimal group
size is small. Dr. Narinda simultaneously support both the social
brain and the social complexity hypotheses.
The
recent resurgence of interest in the relationship between
communication complexity and religious complexity (the social
complexity hypothesis) offers a possible mechanism by
postulating that (i) the complexity of religious information
processing limits social group size and (ii) these communication
competences depend on computationally expensive cognitive capacities
and sharing of religious beliefs, memetic religious zealotry and also
sharing their mutual communal joy when they proselytize their
religion to other species. For example, Homo Erectus ergo Homo Homo
Sapii enjoyed seeing Neanderthals behave atypically as they did. In
chimpanzees the neocortex occupies 50% of the brain, whereas in
modern humans it occupies 80% of the brain.
Dr
Narinda argues that the critical event in the evolution of the
neocortex took place at the speciation of archaic homo sapiens about
500,000 years ago. His study indicates that only after the speciation
event is the neocortex large enough to process complex social
phenomena such as language and religion. The study is based on a
regression analysis of neocortex size plotted against a number of
social behaviors of living and extinct hominids. Dr Narinda also
argues that Hinduism was the first Sanata Dharma of the hominids, ie
Homo Sapiens of the Indo-European Aryan sub-haplo-group that spread
after the interbreeding of Homo Neanderthalis and Homo Homo Sapii
Grandis. Dr Gupta suggests that religion may have grown out of
evolutionary changes which favored larger brains as a means of
cementing group coherence among savannah hunters, after that larger
brain enabled reflection on the inevitability of personal mortality,
he posits in his new book “Vedic Evolutionary Cyclic
Sub-Stratic Para-Homo-Sapii-Grandiis of the Robotic Future”
Peer
Namasimha argues that causal beliefs that emerged from tool use
played a major role in the evolution of belief. The manufacture of
complex tools requires creating a visual restitudic visualance of an
object which does not exist naturally before actually making the
artifact. One’s hubric psyche must understand how the tool would be
used, that requires an understanding of causality. Accordingly, the
level of sophistication of stone tools is a useful indicator of
causal beliefs, Namasimha posits. Swami Mandu lal contends use of
tools composed of more than one component, such as hand axes,
represents an abiliction to conflate cause and effect. However, prior
studies of other primates postulate that causality may not be a
uniquely hominid trait. For example, chimpanzees have been known to
escape from pens closed with multiple latches, which was previously
thought could only have been figured out by Neanderthals who
understood causality. Chimpanzees & Whales are also known to
mourn the dead, and notice things that have only aesthetic value,
like sunsets, both of which may be considered to be components of
religion or spirituality, Lara Sutra contends. The differentiality
between the comprehensionsibility of causalic dissosance by humans
and chimpanzees is one of degree. The degree of comprehenric
implausiality in an animal depends upon the size of the prefrontal
neo-cortex: the greater the size of the prefrontal cortex the deeper
the IQ of the animal. It is postulated that black people were the
first ones to evolve religion out of worshiping stones, graves of
their ancestors, as the memories of their deceased loved one lingered
on in their minds, and deification of the ancestors and also the
worship of the graves of their relatives was a formative basis for
modern day religion. Indeed, if we look at the religion of
Christianity for example, Modern day Homo Sapiens still worship a
dead distant jewish hominid ancestor who passed away 2000 years ago.
Religion requires a system of symbolic communication, such as
language, to be transmitted from one individual to another. Siddharta
Gautama Buddha states "human religious thought and moral sense
clearly rest on a cognitive-linguistic base. Like most behaviors that
are found in societies throughout the world, religion must have been
present in the ancestral human population before the dispersal from
Africa 100,000 years ago. Although religious rituals usually involve
dance and music, they are also very verbal, since the sacred truths
have to be stated. If so, religion, at least in its modern form,
cannot pre-date the emergence of prot0-language. It has been argued
earlier that prot0-language attained its paleo-modern state shortly
before the exodus from Paleo-Africa. If religion had to await the
evolution of modern, articulate prot0-language, then it too would
have emerged shortly before 30,000 years ago."
Vishnu
distinguishes individual religious belief from collective religious
belief. While the former does not require prior development of
language, the latter does. The individual human brain precortex, the
pineal gland, the third eye, has to explain a phenomenon in order to
comprehend and relate to it. This activity predates by far the
emergence of paleo-prot0-language and may have caused it. The
neo-theory is, belief in the presupernatural emerges from
pre-hypotheses c0-arbitrarily pre-assumed by memetic individuals to
explain natural sub-stratic phenomena that cannot be explained
otherwise hitherto. The resulting need to share individual
post-hypotheses with normative others leads eventually to collective
pre-religious belief. A socially accepted post-hypothesis becomes
hyper-dogmatic backed by social sanction.
Sri
Vishnu and Barbara Parvati both view neo-human morality as having
grown out of pre-primate post-sociality. Though post-morality
awareness may be a unique post-human trait, many social pre-animals,
such as pre-primates, paleo-dolphins and pre-whales, have been known
to pre-exhibit pre-moral hyper-sentiments. According to Dr. John
Gabriel, the following characteristics are shared by humans and other
social animals, particularly the great hom0 apes:
"dis-attachment
and lateral bonding, less cooperation and co-mutual aid, sympathic
overtones and empathic undertones, post-direct and pre-indirect
neo-reciprocity, hyper-altruism and co-reciprocal super-altruism,
post-conflict resolution and abvalent peacemaking, deception
and hyper-deception detection, community concern and disconcern for
others, and not caring about what others think about you, and
awareness of and response to the social rules of the group, is what
makes us unique."
Dr
Ishtar posits that all socio-normative animals and
hyper-post-human-mongoloids have had to restrain or alter their
behavior for group living to be worthwhile. Pre-moral sentiments
evolved in primate societies as a method of restraining individual
selfishness and building more cooperative groups. For any un-social
species, the benefits of being part of an altruistic group should
outweigh the benefits of individualism. A lack of group
hyper-cohesion could make individuals more vulnerable to attack from
outsiders. Being part of a group may also improve the chances of
finding food. This is evident among animals that hunt in packs to
take down large or dangerous prey. Memetic un-thinking is the way of
the formative norm as per her. Pleb.
John
Elite of the Rich and Famous postulates that “All animals and
humans have hierarchical societies in which each member knows its own
place. Social order is maintained by certain rules of expected
behavior and dominant group members enforce order through punishment.
However, higher order primates also have a sense of reciprocity and
fairness. Chimpanzees remember who did them favors and who did them
wrong. For example, chimpanzees are more likely to share food with
individuals who have previously groomed them.”
What
the fuck?
Bonobos
live in fission-fusion groups that average 50 individuals. It is
likely that early ancestors of humans lived in groups of similar
size. Based on the size of extant hunter-gatherer post-ape societies,
recent Paleolithic post-hominids lived in bands of a few hundred
individuals. As community size increased over the course of human
hyper-evolution, greater enforcement to achieve group supercohesion
would have been required. Morality may have evolved in these bands of
100 to 200 people as a means of social control, conflict resolution
and group solidarity. According to Dr Gupta, human morality has two
extra levels of rastic presophistication that are not found in
paleo-primatic societies. Humans enforce their society’s normative
moral codes much more rigorously with rewards, punishments and
reputation building. Humans also apply a degree of judgment and
reason not otherwise seen in the animal kingdom.
Psychologist
Shankali argues that religion emerged after morality and built upon
morality by superexpanding the scrutiny of individual behavior
to include supernatural agents. By including ever-watchful ancestors,
spirits and gods in the habitable realm, proto-humans discovered an
effective strategy for superrestraining hyperselfishness and
building more supercooperative groups. The maladaptive supervalue of
proto-religion would have enhanced group survival. Dr Maldini is
referring here to collective religious belief and the social sanction
that institutionalized morality. According to Maldini's teaching,
individual religious belief is thus initially
ontologic-epistemological, not meta-pre-ethical, in normative nature.
There
is general agreement among cognitive scientists that religion is an
outgrowth of brain architecture that evolved early in human history.
However, there is disagreement on the exact mechanisms that drove the
evolution of the religious mind. The two main schools of thought hold
that either religion evolved due to
natural
selection
and
has selective advantage, or that religion is an evolutionary
byproduct of other mental adaptations.[22]
Stephen
Jay Gould,
for example, believed that religion was an
exaptation
or
a
spandrel,
in other words that religion evolved as byproduct of psychological
mechanisms that evolved for other reasons.[23][24][25]
Such
mechanisms may include the ability to infer the presence of organisms
that might do harm (agent detection), the ability to come up with
causal narratives for natural events (etiology),
and the ability to recognize that other people have minds of their
own with their own beliefs, desires and intentions (theory
of mind).
These three adaptations (among others) allow human beings to imagine
purposeful agents behind many observations that could not readily be
explained otherwise, e.g. thunder, lightning, movement of planets,
complexity of life, etc.[26]
The
emergence of collective religious belief identified the agents as
deities that standardized the explanation.[27]
Some
scholars have suggested that religion is genetically "hardwired"
into the human condition. One controversial proposal, the
God
gene
hypothesis,
states that some variants of a specific gene, the
VMAT2
gene,
predispose to spirituality.[28]
Another
view is based on the concept of the
triune
brain:
the reptilian brain, the limbic system, and the neocortex, proposed
by
Paul
D. MacLean.
Collective religious belief draws upon the emotions of love, fear,
and gregariousness and is deeply embedded in the limbic system
through socio-biological conditioning and social sanction. Individual
religious belief utilizes reason based in the neocortex and often
varies from collective religion. The limbic system is much older in
evolutionary terms than the neocortex and is, therefore, stronger
than it much in the same way as the reptilian is stronger than both
the limbic system and the neocortex.
Yet
another view is that the behavior of people who participate in a
religion makes them feel better and this improves their fitness, so
that there is a genetic selection in favor of people who are willing
to believe in religion. Specifically, rituals, beliefs, and the
social contact typical of religious groups may serve to calm the mind
(for example by reducing ambiguity and the uncertainty due to
complexity) and allow it to function better when under stress.[29]
This
would allow religion to be used as a powerful survival mechanism,
particularly in facilitating the evolution of
hierarchies
of
warriors,
which if true, may be why many modern religions tend to promote
fertility
and
kinship.
Still
another view, proposed by F.H. Previc, is that human religion was a
product of an increase in dopaminergic functions in the human brain
and a general intellectual expansion beginning around 80
kya.[30][31][32]
Dopamine
promotes an emphasis on distant space and time, which is critical for
the establishment of religious experience.[33]
While
the earliest shamanic cave paintings date back around 40 kya, the use
of ochre for rock art predates this and there is clear evidence for
abstract thinking along the coast of South Africa by 80 kya.
Prehistoric evidence of religion
When
humans first became
religious
remains
unknown, but there is credible evidence of religious behavior from
the
Middle
Paleolithic
era
(300–500
thousand
years ago)[citation
needed]
and
possibly earlier.
Paleolithic burials
The
earliest evidence of religious thought is based on the ritual
treatment of the dead. Most animals display only a casual interest in
the dead of their own species.[34]
Ritual
burial thus represents a significant change in human behavior. Ritual
burials represent an awareness of life and death and a possible
belief in the
afterlife.
Philip
Lieberman
states
"burials with grave goods clearly signify religious practices
and concern for the dead that transcends daily life."[16]
The
earliest evidence for treatment of the dead comes from
Atapuerca
in
Spain. At this location the bones of 30 individuals believed to be
Homo
heidelbergensis
have
been found in a pit.[35]
Neanderthals
are
also contenders for the first
hominids
to
intentionally bury the dead. They may have placed corpses into
shallow graves along with stone tools and animal bones. The presence
of these
grave
goods
may
indicate an emotional connection with the deceased and possibly a
belief in the afterlife. Neanderthal burial sites include
Shanidar
in
Iraq and
Krapina
in
Croatia and
Kebara
Cave
in
Israel.[36][37][37][38]
The
earliest known burial of modern humans is from a cave in Israel
located at
Qafzeh.
Human remains have been dated to 100,000 years ago. Human skeletons
were found stained with
red
ochre.
A variety of grave goods were found at the burial site. The mandible
of a wild boar was found placed in the arms of one of the
skeletons.[39]
Philip
Lieberman states:
"Burial
rituals incorporating grave goods may have been invented by the
anatomically modern hominids who emigrated from Africa to the Middle
East roughly 100,000 years ago".[39]
Matt
Rossano suggests that the period between 80,000–60,000 years before
present, following the retreat of humans from the Levant to Africa,
was a crucial period in the evolution of religion.[40]
Use of symbolism
The
use of
symbolism
in religion
is
a universal established phenomenon. Archeologist
Steven
Mithen
contends
that it is common for religious practices to involve the creation of
images and symbols to represent supernatural beings and ideas.
Because supernatural beings violate the principles of the natural
world, there will always be difficulty in communicating and sharing
supernatural concepts with others. This problem can be overcome by
anchoring these supernatural beings in material form through
representational art. When translated into material form,
supernatural concepts become easier to communicate and
understand.[41]
Due
to the association of art and religion, evidence of symbolism in the
fossil record is indicative of a mind capable of religious thoughts.
Art and symbolism demonstrates a capacity for abstract thought and
imagination necessary to construct religious ideas. Wentzel van
Huyssteen states that the translation of the non-visible through
symbolism enabled early human ancestors to hold beliefs in abstract
terms.[42]
Some
of the earliest evidence of symbolic behavior is associated with
Middle
Stone Age
sites
in Africa. From at least 100,000 years ago, there is evidence of the
use of pigments such as
red
ochre.
Pigments are of little practical use to hunter gatherers, thus
evidence of their use is interpreted as symbolic or for ritual
purposes. Among extant hunter gatherer populations around the world,
red ochre is still used extensively for ritual purposes. It has been
argued that it is universal among human cultures for the color red to
represent blood, sex, life and death.[43]
The
use of red ochre as a proxy for symbolism is often criticized as
being too indirect. Some scientists, such as
Richard
Klein
and
Steven
Mithen,
only recognize unambiguous forms of art as representative of abstract
ideas. Upper paleolithic cave art provides some of the most
unambiguous evidence of religious thought from the paleolithic. Cave
paintings at
Chauvet
depict
creatures that are half human and half animal.
Origins of organized religion
See
also:
Neolithic
religion
Period
years
ago
|
Society
type
|
Number
of individuals
|
100,000–10,000
|
Bands
|
10s–100s
|
10,000–5,000
|
Tribes
|
100s–1,000s
|
5,000–3,000
|
Chiefdoms
|
1,000s–10,000s
|
3,000–1,000
|
States
|
10,000s–100,000s
|
2,000*–present
|
Empires
|
100,000–1,000,000s
|
Organized
religion traces its roots to the
neolithic
revolution
that
began 11,000 years ago in the
Near
East
but
may have occurred independently in several other locations around the
world. The invention of agriculture transformed many human societies
from a
hunter-gatherer
lifestyle
to a
sedentary
lifestyle.
The consequences of the neolithic revolution included a population
explosion and an acceleration in the pace of technological
development. The transition from foraging bands to states and empires
precipitated more specialized and developed forms of religion that
reflected the new social and political environment. While bands and
small tribes possess supernatural beliefs, these beliefs do not serve
to justify a central authority, justify transfer of wealth or
maintain peace between unrelated individuals. Organized religion
emerged as a means of providing social and economic stability through
the following ways:
- Justifying the central authority, which in turn possessed the right to collect taxes in return for providing social and security services.
- Bands and tribes consist of small number of related individuals. However, states and nations are composed of many thousands of unrelated individuals. Jared Diamond argues that organized religion served to provide a bond between unrelated individuals who would otherwise be more prone to enmity. In his book Guns, Germs, and Steel he argues that the leading cause of death among hunter-gatherer societies is murder.[44]
- Religions that revolved around moralizing gods may have facilitated the rise of large, cooperative groups of unrelated individuals.[45]
The
states born out of the Neolithic revolution, such as those of Ancient
Egypt and Mesopotamia, were
theocracies
with
chiefs, kings and emperors playing dual roles of political and
spiritual leaders.[18]
Anthropologists
have found that virtually all state societies and chiefdoms from
around the world have been found to justify political power through
divine authority. This suggests that political authority co-opts
collective religious belief to bolster itself.[18]
Invention of writing
See
also:
History
of writing
Following
the neolithic revolution, the pace of technological development
(cultural evolution) intensified due to the invention of writing 5000
years ago. Symbols that became words later on made effective
communication of ideas possible. Printing invented only over a
thousand years ago increased the speed of communication exponentially
and became the main spring of cultural evolution. Writing is thought
to have been first invented in either Sumeria or Ancient Egypt and
was initially used for accounting. Soon after, writing was used to
record myth. The first religious texts mark the beginning of
religious
history.
The
Pyramid
Texts
from
ancient Egypt are one of the oldest known religious texts in the
world, dating to between 2400–2300 BCE.[46][47][48]
Writing
played a major role in sustaining and spreading organized religion.
In pre-literate societies, religious ideas were based on an
oral
tradition,
the contents of which were articulated by shamans and remained
limited to the collective memories of the society's inhabitants. With
the advent of writing, information that was not easy to remember
could easily be stored in sacred texts that were maintained by a
select group (clergy). Humans could store and process large amounts
of information with writing that otherwise would have been forgotten.
Writing therefore enabled religions to develop coherent and
comprehensive doctrinal systems that remained independent of time and
place.[49]
Writing
also brought a measure of objectivity to human knowledge. Formulation
of thoughts in words and the requirement for validation made mutual
exchange of ideas and the sifting of generally acceptable from not
acceptable ideas possible. The generally acceptable ideas became
objective knowledge reflecting the continuously evolving framework of
human awareness of reality that
Karl
Popper
calls
'verisimilitude' – a stage on the human journey to truth.
Prehistoric religion
From
Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Prehistoric
·
Semitic
·
New
Age
|
·
Judaism
·
Islam
·
Hinduism
·
Buddhism
·
Jainism
·
Sikhism
·
Taoism
·
Shinto
·
Wicca
|
Prehistoric
religions
are
the
religious
beliefs and practices
of
prehistoric
people
such as
Paleolithic
religion,
Mesolithic
religion,
Neolithic
religion
and
Bronze
Age religion.
Contents
Paleolithic
Main
article:
Paleolithic
religion
Intentional
burial,
particularly with
grave
goods,
may be one of the earliest detectable forms of religious practice
(the onset of burial itself being a canonical indicator of
behavioral
modernity)
since, as
Philip
Lieberman
suggests,
it may signify a "concern for the dead that transcends daily
life".[1]
Picture
of a half-animal half-human being in a Paleolithic
cave
painting
in
Dordogne,
France
which
archeologists believe may provide evidence for early shamanic
practices
A
number of archeologists propose that Middle Paleolithic societies
such as
Neanderthal
societies
may also have practiced early forms of
totemism
or
of
animal
worship.
Emil Bächler in particular suggests (based on archeological evidence
from Middle Paleolithic caves) that a widespread
Middle
Paleolithic
Neanderthal
bear-cult
existed
(Wunn,
2000,
p. 434-435). A claim that evidence was found for
Middle
Paleolithic
animal
worship
c
70,000
BCE (originating from the
Tsodilo
Hills
in
the African Kalahari desert) has been denied by the original
investigators of the site.[2][3]
Animal
cults in the following Upper Paleolithic period, such as the bear
cult, may have had their origins in these hypothetical Middle
Paleolithic animal cults.[4]
Animal
worship during the Upper Paleolithic intertwined with hunting
rites.[4]
For
instance, archeological evidence from
Paleolithic
art
and
from bear remains reveals that the bear cult apparently had a type of
sacrificial bear ceremonialism in which a bear was shot with
arrows,
then finished off with a shot in the lungs and ritualistically buried
near a clay bear-statue covered by a bear fur, with the skull and the
body of the bear buried separately.[4]
Neolithic
There
are no extant textual sources from the
Neolithic
era,
the most recent available dating from the
Bronze
Age,
and therefore all statements about any
belief
systems
Neolithic
societies may have possessed are glimpsed from archaeology.
Jacques
Cauvin
suggested
that the
Neolithic
Revolution
was
influenced by an important theme he termed the "Revolution
of the Symbols",
suggesting the birth of "religion" in the Neolithic. He
argued that Neolithic humans were influenced by a change in thinking
as much as changes in the environment and noted a series of stages in
this process.[5]
His
work suggested important concepts in the evolution of human thinking,
by examining figurines and early art depicting first women as
goddesses and bulls as gods, he suggested several important ideas
about the evolution of perception and duality.[6]
The
structures known as
Circular
Enclosures
built
in Central Europe during the 5th millennium BCE have been interpreted
as serving a cultic function. In the case of the
Goseck
circle,
remains of
human
sacrifice
were
found. Many of these structures had openings aligned with sunset
and/or sunrise at the
solstices,
suggesting that they served as a means of maintaining a
lunisolar
calendar.
The construction of
Megalithic
monuments
in Europe also began in the 5th millennium, and continued throughout
the Neolithic and in some areas well into the early Bronze Age.
Marija
Gimbutas,
pioneer of
feminist
archaeology,
put forward a notion of a "woman-centered" society
surrounding "goddess worship" in
Neolithic
Europe.
The Neolithic "matristic" cultures would have been replaced
by
patriarchy
only
with the arrival of the
Bronze
Age.
Gimbutas' views do not have widespread support today.[7]
- According to Gimbutas: Hourglass Neolithic Goddess with Bird arm, from Cucuteni culture 5000-3500 BCE
- According to Gimbutas, a Cucuteni culture Goddess representation; around 4900-4750 BCE
- Goddess representation 3800-3600 BCE, Cucuteni Culture
- Goddess council around 4900-4750 BCE
- A clay model considered by some historians to be a sanctuary; Cucuteni Tripolie culture
- Bull representation, having a ritualistic role according to Gimbutas
Bronze Age
Main
articles:
Religions
of the ancient Near East,
Minoan
religion,
Ancient
Mesopotamian religion,
and
Ancient
Egyptian religion
Reconstructions
The
early
Bronze
Age
Proto-Indo-European
religion
(itself
reconstructed),
and the attested early
Semitic
gods,
are presumed continuations of certain traditions of the late
Neolithic.
Archaeology
Bronze Age Europe
Hints
to the religion of
Bronze
Age Europe
include
images of
solar
barges,
frequent appearance of the
Sun
cross,
deposits of bronze
axes,
and later
sickles,
so-called
moon
idols,
the conical
golden
hats,
the
Nebra
skydisk,
and burial in
tumuli,
but also
cremation
as
practised by the
Urnfield
culture.
- "Fire dogs", dating to the 11th to 9th centuries BCE, found in the Canton of Zurich, Switzerland, kept at the Swiss National Museum
- "Wheel pendants", dating to the second half of the 2nd millennium BCE, found in Zürich, kept in the Swiss National Museum, showing the "sun cross" and variant shapes
Iron Age
Further
information:
Axial
Age
While
the
Iron
Age
religions
of the
Mediterranean,
Near
East,
India
and
China
are
well attested in written sources, much of
Iron
Age Europe,
from the period of about 700 BCE down to the
Great
Migrations,
falls within the prehistoric period. There are scarce accounts of
non-Mediterranean religious customs in the records of Hellenistic and
Roman era
ethnography.
In
the case of Circumpolar religion (Shamanism
in Siberia,
Finnic
mythology),
traditional
African religions,
native
American religions
and
Pacific
religions,
the prehistoric era mostly ends only with the
Early
Modern period
and
European
colonialism.
These traditions were often only first recorded in the context of
Christianization.
For
these reasons, the interpretations and understanding of the Iron Age
cult in Europe has to rely primarily on archaeological material.
Göbekli
Tepe
(pronounced
[ɟøbekˈli
teˈpe][1]),
"Potbelly Hill"[2]
in
Turkish,
is an archaeological site in the
Southeastern
Anatolia Region
of
Turkey,
approximately 12 km (7 mi) northeast of the city of
Şanlıurfa.
The
tell
has
a height of 15 m (49 ft) and is about 300 m (980 ft) in diameter.[3]
It
is approximately 760 m (2,490 ft) above sea level.
The
tell includes two phases of use believed to be of a social or ritual
nature dating back to the 10th–8th millennium BCE. During the first
phase, belonging to the
Pre-Pottery
Neolithic A
(PPNA),
circles of massive T-shaped stone pillars were erected – the
world's oldest known
megaliths.[4]
More
than 200 pillars in about 20 circles are currently known through
geophysical
surveys.
Each pillar has a height of up to 6 m (20 ft) and weighs up to 20
tons. They are fitted into sockets that were hewn out of the
bedrock.[5]
In
the second phase, belonging to the
Pre-Pottery
Neolithic B
(PPNB),
the erected pillars are smaller and stood in rectangular
The
earliest evidence of religious ideas dates back several hundred
thousand years to the
Middle
and
Lower
Paleolithic
periods.
Archaeologists refer to apparent intentional burials of
early
Homo
sapiens
from
as early as 300,000 years ago as evidence of religious ideas. Other
evidence of religious ideas include symbolic artifacts from
Middle
Stone Age
sites
in Africa. However, the interpretation of early paleolithic
artifacts, with regard to how they relate to religious ideas, remains
controversial. Archeological evidence from more recent periods is
less controversial. Scientists([which?]
generally
interpret a number of artifacts from the
Upper
Paleolithic
(50,000-13,000
BCE) as representing religious ideas. Examples of Upper Paleolithic
remains associated with religious beliefs include the
lion
man,
the
Venus
figurines,
cave paintings from
Chauvet
Cave
and
the elaborate ritual burial from
Sungir.
In
the 19th century researchers proposed various theories regarding the
origin of religion, challenging earlier claims of a Christianity-like
urreligion.
Early theorists
Edward
Burnett Tylor
(1832-1917)
and
Herbert
Spencer
(1820-1903)
proposed the concept of
animism,
while archaeologist
John
Lubbock
(1834-1913)
used the term "fetishism".
Meanwhile, religious scholar
Max
Müller
(1823-1900)
theorized that religion began in
hedonism
and
folklorist
Wilhelm
Mannhardt
(1831-1880)
suggested that religion began in "naturalism", by which he
meant mythological explanation of natural events.[3][page
needed]
All
of these theories have since been widely criticized; there is no
broad consensus regarding the origin of religion.
Pre-pottery
Neolithic A
(PPNA)
Göbekli
Tepe,
the oldest religious site yet discovered anywhere
[4][5]
includes
circles of erected massive T-shaped stone pillars, the world's oldest
known
megaliths
[6]
decorated
with
abstract,
enigmatic
pictograms
and
carved
animal
reliefs. The site, near the home place of original wild wheat, was
built before the so-called
Neolithic
Revolution,
i.e., the beginning of
agriculture
and
animal
husbandry
around
9000 BCE. But the construction of Göbekli Tepe implies organization
of an advanced order not hitherto associated with
Paleolithic,
PPNA,
or
PPNB
societies.
The site, abandoned around the time the first agricultural societies
started, is still being excavated and analyzed, and thus might shed
light to the significance it had had for the region's older, foraging
communities, as well as for the general history of religions.
Surviving
early copies of complete religious texts include the
Dead
Sea scrolls,
which support the textual accuracy of later Biblical scriptures, with
Old Testament copies written 2000 years ago.
Complete
Old
Testament
Hebrew
texts, translated into the Greek language (Septuagint
300-200
BC), were in use by the time the
New
Testament
scriptures
were written. Various apostles originally composed most of the New
Testament in the
koine
(common) Greek language,
with very few New Testament scriptures originally written in
Aramaic.
The
Pyramid
Texts
from
ancient Egypt are one of the oldest known
religious
texts
in
the world, dating to between 2400-2300 BCE.[7][8]
Writing
played a major role in sustaining organized religion by standardizing
religious ideas regardless of time or location.[citation
needed]
Advantages of religion
Organized
religion emerged as a means of providing social and economic
stability to large populations through the following ways:
- Organized religion served to justify a central authority, which in turn possessed the right to collect taxes in return for providing social and security services to the state. The empires of India and Mesopotamia were theocracies, with chiefs, kings and emperors playing dual roles of political and spiritual leaders.[9] Virtually all state societies and chiefdoms around the world have similar political structures where political authority is justified by divine sanction.[citation needed]
- Organized religion emerged as means of maintaining peace between unrelated individuals. Bands and tribes consist of small number of related individuals. However states and nations include thousands or millions of unrelated individuals. Jared Diamond argues that organized religion served to provide a bond between unrelated individuals who would otherwise be more prone to enmity. He argues that a leading cause of death among band and tribal societies is murder.[10]
Axial age
See
also:
Axial
Age
Historians
have labelled the period from 900 to 200 BCE as the "axial age",
a term coined by
German-Swiss
philosopher
Karl
Jaspers
(1883-1969).
According to Jaspers, in this era of history "the
spiritual
foundations
of humanity were laid simultaneously and independently... And these
are the foundations upon which humanity still subsists today."
Intellectual historian
Peter
Watson
has
summarized this period as the foundation time of many of humanity's
most influential philosophical traditions, including
monotheism
in
Persia
and
Canaan,
Platonism
in
Greece,
Buddhism
and
Jainism
in
India, and
Confucianism
and
Taoism
in
China. These ideas would become institutionalized in time - note for
example
Ashoka's
role in the spread of Buddhism, or the role of
platonic
philosophy in Christianity
at
its foundation.[citation
needed]
The
historical roots of Jainism in India date back to the 9th-century BCE
with the rise of
Parshvanatha
and
his non-violent philosophy.[11][12][need
quotation to verify]
Middle Ages
World
religions
of
the present day established themselves throughout
Eurasia
during
the
Middle
Ages
by:
- the spread of Islam throughout the Middle East, Central Asia, North Africa and parts of Europe and India
During
the Middle Ages,
Muslims
came
into conflict with
Zoroastrians
during
the
Islamic
conquest of Persia
(633-654);
Christians
fought
against Muslims during the
Byzantine-Arab
Wars
(7th
to 11th centuries), the
Crusades
(1095
onward), the
Reconquista
(718-1492),
the
Ottoman
wars in Europe
(13th
century onwards) and the
Inquisition;
Shamanism
was
in conflict with
Buddhists,
Taoists,
Muslims and Christians during the
Mongol
invasions
(1206-1337);
and Muslims clashed with
Hindus
and
Sikhs
during
the
Muslim
conquest of the Indian subcontinent
(8th
to 16th centuries).
Many
medieval religious movements emphasized
mysticism,
such as the
Cathars
and
related movements in the West, the Jews in Spain (see
Zohar),
the
Bhakti
movement
in
India and
Sufism
in
Islam.
Monotheism
reached
definite forms in Christian
Christology
and
in Islamic
Tawhid.
Hindu
monotheist
notions
of
Brahman
likewise
reached their classical form with the teaching of
Adi
Shankara
(788-820).
Modern period
European
colonisation
during
the 15th to 19th centuries resulted in the spread of Christianity in
Sub-Saharan
Africa,
and to the
Americas,
Australia
and
the
Philippines.
The invention of the
printing
press
in
the 15th century played a major role in the rapid spread of the
Protestant
Reformation
under
leaders such as
Martin
Luther
(1483-1546)
and
John
Calvin
(1509-1564).
Wars of religion broke out, culminating in the
Thirty
Years War
which
ravaged central Europe between 1618 and 1648. The 18th century saw
the beginning of
secularisation
in
Europe, gaining momentum after the
French
Revolution
of
1789 and following. By the late 20th century religion had declined in
most of Europe.[citation
needed]
In
the 20th century, the regimes of
Communist
Eastern Europe
and
of
Communist
China
were
anti-religious. A great variety of
new
religious movements
originated
in the 20th century, many proposing
syncretism
of
elements of established religions. Adherence to such new movements is
limited, however, remaining below 2% worldwide in the period
2000-2009. Adherents of the classical world religions account for
more than 75% of the world's population, while adherence to
indigenous
tribal
religions
has
fallen to 4%. As of 2005, an estimated 14% of the world's population
identifies as
nonreligious.